Next Article in Journal
Episodic Memory Performance Modifies the Strength of the Age–Brain Structure Relationship
Previous Article in Journal
Adaptation and Validation of the Lithuanian-Language Version of the Olympic Value Scale (OVS)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Life Conditions as Mediators of Welfare State Effect on Mental Wellbeing among Oldest Old in Europe

by
Johanna Cresswell-Smith
1,*,
Kristian Wahlbeck
2 and
Jorid Kalseth
3
1
Faculty of Medicine, University of Helsinki, P.O. Box 63, 00014 Helsinki, Finland
2
Mental Health Team, Finnish Institute for Health and Welfare, P.O. Box 30, 00271 Helsinki, Finland
3
SINTEF Digital, P.O. Box 4760, 7465 Trondheim, Norway
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(7), 4363; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074363
Submission received: 1 February 2022 / Revised: 29 March 2022 / Accepted: 1 April 2022 / Published: 5 April 2022

Abstract

:
Background: Mental wellbeing is formed by our daily environments, which are, in turn, influenced by public policies, such as the welfare state. This paper looks at how different aspects of life conditions may mediate the welfare state effect on mental wellbeing in oldest old age. Methods: Data were extracted from Round 6 of the European Social Survey (2012). The dataset comprised of 2058 people aged 80 years and older from 24 countries. Mediation analyses determined possible links between the welfare state, including eleven intervening variables representing life conditions and five mental wellbeing dimensions. Results: Our study confirms that the higher the level of welfare state, the better mental wellbeing, irrespective of dimension. Although several life conditions were found to mediate the welfare state effect on mental wellbeing, subjective general health, coping with income and place in society were the most important intervening variables. Conclusions: All three variables centre around supporting autonomy in the oldest old age. By teasing out how the welfare state influences mental wellbeing in the oldest old, we can better understand the many drivers of wellbeing and enable evidence informed age-friendly policy making.

1. Introduction

The oldest old population, defined here as adults aged 80 years and over [1] is projected to rise globally from 143 million in 2019 to 426 million by 2050 [2]. This demographic transition has stimulated broad discussions regarding the need for age-friendly environments [3]. Mental wellbeing is an important area of focus in this regard, with links to positive health outcomes [4], increased life expectancy [5] and economic benefits [6,7,8]. Our daily environments are shaped by the surrounding welfare state policies, which in turn, impact mental wellbeing also in oldest old age [9].
Definitions of mental wellbeing differ according to which distinct features and dimensions are emphasised [10]. Subjective wellbeing [11] approaches tend to emphasise the evaluative dimension of wellbeing with a focus on cognitive appraisals of life satisfaction, together with the hedonic or emotional dimension, which relates to a sense of pleasure and happiness [12,13,14]. Eudaimonic approaches, on the other hand, emphasise the importance of meaning, for example, via personal development and self-realisation. These approaches include Ryff’s definition of psychological wellbeing made up of life purpose, mastery and positive relatedness, flow, autonomy, personal growth and self-acceptance [15].
Keyes’ model [16,17] is built in relation to factors that influence flourishing and languishing, while Seligman’s PERMA model builds on five components, including positive emotions, engagement, relationships, meaning and accomplishment [18]. Approaching mental wellbeing as a multidimensional construct allows for a comprehensive understanding [19,20].
Exploring different dimensions of mental wellbeing sheds light on phenomenon, such as the ‘wellbeing paradox’, denoting a tendency for the evaluative dimension to increase in older age, particularly in the context of wealthier, English speaking countries [21]. Socio-economic or socio-political context appears to lie behind this paradox, as the effect has not been replicated in lower income countries or in post-communist countries [22]. Different aspects of wellbeing tend to vary according to age group and what dimensions of wellbeing are under consideration as well as on the individual characteristics and life circumstances of the study participants [23]. Furthermore, specific aspects of a country’s social policy have been found to influence wellbeing [23].
For example, high levels of emotional wellbeing (happiness) have been attributed to the influence of welfare state in Nordic countries [24] as have high levels of flourishing [25]. Wellbeing may be influenced by characteristics, such as low income inequality, high social trust and well-developed social welfare and health care systems [26]. These approaches parallel research on the importance of social determinants of mental health [27]. In line with this, the Mental Wellbeing Impact Assessment (MWIA) framework proposes four protective pathways for mental wellbeing—namely enhancing control, increasing resilience and community assets, facilitating participation and promoting inclusion [28].
Despite constituting a growing segment of the population, the oldest old age group tends to be overlooked in research [29,30]. In a step towards ameliorating this research gap, the European Welfare Models and Mental Wellbeing in Final Years of Life (EMMY) project (www.emmyproject.eu) used a mixed-methods approach to explore mental wellbeing in the oldest old age group. The EMMY project (2017–2019) made use of data from Round 6 of the European Social Survey (ESS). These included data from rotating modules on personal and social wellbeing and a broad range of items related to MWB [31].
Exploratory factor analysis [32] highlighted five important dimensions of mental wellbeing in oldest old age (1) the evaluative dimension capturing overall happiness and life-satisfaction; (2) the emotional dimension including enjoyment, calmness and happiness; (3) positive psychological functioning, built on autonomy, competence, self-esteem and optimism; (4) meaning and flow, describing states of presence and engagement; and (5) positive and supportive relationships that is, giving and receiving support and feeling appreciated [33]. A further paper originating from the EMMY project found that a welfare state mattered more for mental wellbeing in older age groups compared to younger age groups [9].
The current paper explores how the conditions of daily life influence the different dimensions of mental wellbeing in the oldest old age. More specifically we look at the mediating effect of these variables on the welfare state effect in terms of five dimensions of mental wellbeing. Teasing out the influence of these variables gives us a better understanding of the drivers of mental wellbeing in oldest old age, facilitating evidence to inform policy making for this growing population group.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. The Source of Data and Variables

Data was extracted from Round 6 of the ESS (2012), which is collected on a broad array of topics via face-to-face interviews [31]. Data were restricted to individuals aged 80 years and older from 24 countries belonging to either the European Union or the European Free Trade Association. Both core and rotating ESS modules were included in this study—namely: Subjective Wellbeing, Social Exclusion, Religion, National and Ethnic Identity, Personal and Social Wellbeing, Media and Social Trust, Politics, as well as Socio demographics [31]. These modules measure different aspects of mental wellbeing, some of which are not available from later surveys waves. Specific items from these modules were compiled to form the mental wellbeing variables, as well as being used as intervening variables.
Mental wellbeing variables included the evaluative, emotional, psychological functioning, flow and relational dimensions [33] (see Table A1). The evaluative dimension of wellbeing refers to a sense of being satisfied with life and the evaluation of a positive affect, such as enjoyment, calmness and happiness. The emotional dimension also includes a feeling of vitality (i.e., being ‘full of energy’). The positive psychological functioning dimension refers to autonomy, competence, self-esteem and optimism. The flow dimension includes the perception of meaning, presence and engagement. The relational dimension consists of positive and supportive relationships, including receiving support and appreciation by others and helping and providing support to others [20]. The values for these dimensions were defined by calculating the average of the individual ESS items that make up a particular dimension. Individual items were first transformed (normalised) to a common scale using a min–max algorithm transforming each item into values between 1 and 5: New score = ((5–1)/(max-min)) * (score-max) + 5.
Intervening variables from the ESS were selected on the basis of their fit with the four MWIA protective factors, i.e., enhancing control, increasing resilience and community assets, facilitating participation and promoting inclusion (see Table A2).

2.2. Welfare State Index (WSI)

Esping-Andersen’s classification of welfare states by social rights and social spending, income redistribution and employment policy is the starting point for many studies investigating welfare state impacts [34,35,36]. This typology has been revised and expanded to include several countries and welfare state-typology variants [37]. However, these regime typologies do not allow for the study of possible variation within these regime types. In line with this, a more flexible composite WSI approach has been used [9] representing different aspects of welfare culture, welfare institutions, policy instruments and outcomes of welfare states [38,39].
The WSI includes the per capita Gross Domestic Product (GDP), health and social expenditures as a percentage of GDP, a gender empowerment measure, life expectancy at 65, work life duration and the Gini coefficient—a measure of individualism and a measure of social trust (see Table A3). The WSI was constructed using factor analysis restricted to one dimension, allowing for different weights for the eight variables. The predicted value of this factor was then used to represent the WSI in the analyses. The WSI correlates highly with the welfare state regime typology as illustrated in our previous study of wellbeing [9].

2.3. Statistical Analyses

Mediation analyses were applied in order to determine possible links between WSI, intervening (mediator) variables and mental wellbeing dimensions, as depicted in Figure 1.
Multilevel analysis were used to analyse the WSI impact on mental wellbeing, taking into account potential unobservable effects applying to individuals from the same country, as well as consequences of the welfare state variable being measured at the country level. Multilevel Structural Equation Modelling (MSEM) was employed to explore potential mediators [40].

The following Steps Were Performed

(1)
An initial analysis of the total effect (path c in Figure 1) of WSI on mental wellbeing entailed five separate regression analyses, one for each mental wellbeing dimension, controlling for age and gender.
(2)
Mediating effects. An intervening variable is considered to be a significant mediator of welfare state effects (i.e., an indirect effect) if it is significantly associated both with WSI (path a in Figure 1) and with mental wellbeing (path b in Figure 1). If the indirect effect (a×b in Figure 1) is equal to zero, this indicates that the direct effect (c’ in Figure 1) equals the total effect (c). The magnitude of the mediation effect can be assessed by comparing coefficients of path c and c’ in Table 3.
MSEM is made up of simultaneous regression of (1) WSI on the intervening variable (path a in Figure 1) and (2) WSI (path c’ in Figure 1) and the mediator variable (path b in Figure 1) on mental wellbeing, taking into account the multilevel structure of the data, i.e., individuals being nested within a country. The product of the coefficients of paths a × b (in Figure 1) was used to test for indirect effects. The effects of all eleven intervening variables were analysed separately meaning a total of 11 * 5 MSEM regression analyses were performed, one for each intervening variable and each mental wellbeing dimension (including control for age and gender).
An intervening variable is deemed a mediator if a significant indirect effect is found, i.e., if the p-value is less than 0.05. A full mediation effect is present when the direct WSI effect (c’ in Figure 1) drops to zero. A partial mediation effect is found if the indirect effect (a × b in Figure 1) is statistically significant, and the direct WSI effect remains significantly different from zero.
All analyses were performed using Stata/SE 16.1. The mixed command was used to analyse the total effect of WSI on mental wellbeing, while the gsem and the postestimation nlcom commands were used in the MSEM regression and to test for indirect effect.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptions

3.1.1. Participants

The dataset comprised 2058 participants, 62% of which were female. The average age was 84.1 years (SD 3.67), and the oldest participant was 103 years of age.

3.1.2. Welfare State Variable

WSI measured at the country level produced values ranging from −1.65 for Bulgaria to 1.69 for Norway. By mapping the WSI scores (see Figure 2) a clear pattern was found with Nordic countries showing the highest WSI scores, followed by the Bismarckian countries, Anglo-Saxon countries, Southern countries and finally the Eastern European countries.

3.2. Intervening Variables

A total of 11 intervening variables (Table 1) were measured using different scales at the individual level; thus, the direct comparison of average scores across items is not possible.

3.3. Mental Wellbeing Variables

The normalised scores for individual mental wellbeing dimensions are shown in Table 2.

3.3.1. Step 1

The first step in the analyses confirmed the total effect of WSI on mental wellbeing (path c in Figure 1) by way of separate regression analyses for each of the five dimensions controlling for age and gender. Figure 3 (left side panel) shows that the WSI variable is positively and significantly associated with all five dimensions of mental wellbeing. This implies that the higher level of WSI, the higher the level of mental wellbeing for each dimension (see right side panel of Figure 3). Higher levels of WSI correspond to high income countries with more well-developed welfare state mechanisms, in this case the Nordic countries and other north-western European countries.
Furthermore, the association of WSI was found to be highest for the evaluative dimension, followed by the emotional dimension, flow, psychological functioning and finally relational dimensions of mental wellbeing. Country differences show that scores for the evaluation dimension were considerably lower in countries with low WSI values compared to the relational dimension, while scores for the evaluative and relational dimensions were similar in countries with high WSI values.

3.3.2. Step 2

The second step investigated whether the intervening variables mediated the association between WSI and individual dimensions of mental wellbeing. The coefficient for WSI without control for intervening variables (total effect c) is shown in the first row of Table 3. Coefficients with control for each intervening variable are listed underneath (direct effect c’). Further mediation diagrams can be found in Appendix A.
The indirect effect was analysed for all five mental wellbeing dimensions with WSI as the independent variable and the dimensions of mental wellbeing as dependent variables. A series of ESS items representing life conditions were used as intervening variables. All intervening variables, except for chance to show capabilities, were found to mediate a significant indirect effect on all mental wellbeing dimensions. The lack of significant indirect effect for the intervening variable chance to show capabilities was due to a lack of associations with WSI (see Table 1), as the association with mental wellbeing was strong (data not presented). Indirect effects by all other intervening variables were significant at p < 0.01.
Subjective general health, coping with income and place in society produced the largest difference between the c and c’ paths in the mediation model and will therefore be presented in more detail. These intervening variables ranked in the top three coefficients of the WSI (i.e., c–c’) for all mental wellbeing dimensions, with the exception of coping with income ranking fifth for the flow dimension. These three variables also had the highest association with the WSI (see Table 1).
The subjective general health item was obtained by asking participants to rate their own general health on a five-point Likert scale (European Social Survey, 2012) [31]. There was a significant positive relationship between WSI and subjective general health, the second highest association with WSI among all intervening variables (WSI effect = 0.411, p < 0.001, Table 1). The indirect effect for the subjective health variable was significant at p < 0.001 for all dimensions of mental wellbeing (see Table 3) meaning that controlling for subjective general health reduced the welfare state effect on all mental wellbeing dimensions considerably.
Full mediation was found for psychological functioning and relational dimensions, although a strong partial meditation was also found for the other dimensions, for example the welfare state effect on the emotional dimension was reduced from 0.252 (c) to 0.093 (c’). Compared to the other intervening variables, the subjective general health item ranked as the strongest mediator for emotional, psychological functioning and flow dimensions as well as the second strongest mediator for the evaluative dimension and third for the relational dimension.
The coping with income variable was generated by asking participants about their current feelings about their household’s income ranging from “living comfortably” to “very difficult”. A significant positive relationship was found between WSI and coping with income, the highest association with WSI among the intervening variables (WSI effect = 0.574, p < 0.001, see Table 1), and the indirect effect for the coping with income variable was significant at p < 0.001 for all dimensions of mental wellbeing (see Table 3). Full mediation was found for the relational dimension, along with a strong partial meditation for other dimensions. The welfare state effect was for example reduced from 0.402 (c) to 0.232 (c’) on the evaluative dimension. Compared to the other intervening variables, the coping with income variable was the strongest mediator of welfare state effect in terms of the evaluative dimension, second strongest for the emotional and relational dimensions and third strongest for the psychological functioning dimension.
The place in society variable asked participants to categorise how they placed themselves in society on a scale from 0 to 10 with zero representing the bottom of society and ten representing the top of society [32]. There was a significant positive relationship between WSI and place in society, the third highest association with WSI among the intervening variables (WSI effect = 0.364, p < 0.001, Table 1), and the indirect effect for the place in society variable was significant at p < 0.001 for all dimensions of mental wellbeing.
Full mediation was found for the relational dimension, and a strong partial meditation was found for the other dimensions. For example, the welfare state effect on the psychological functioning dimension was reduced from 0.147 (c) to 0.063 (c’). Compared to the other intervening variables, the place in society intervening variable had the strongest mediating effect in terms of the relational dimension; it was the second strongest mediator of the welfare state effect for the psychological functioning and flow dimensions and third for the evaluative and emotional dimensions.

4. Discussion

The results identify important mediators of the welfare state effect on five different dimensions of mental wellbeing in oldest old age. Our study confirms that higher levels of the WSI are associated with higher the levels of different dimensions of mental wellbeing. Furthermore, by exploring how the conditions of daily life mediate the welfare state effect in relation to these dimensions, three areas were found to be particularly influential: subjective general health, coping with income and place in society each impacted specific dimensions of mental wellbeing differently.

4.1. Subjective General Health and Mental Wellbeing

Health is especially important in oldest old age due to the higher likelihood of illness and increased support needs [41]. Older adults are often reliant on support, such as care and pension provisions for their wellbeing, which simultaneously serve to ameliorate health inequalities [42]. The current study found higher levels of subjective general health in stronger welfare states compared with less developed welfare states. The welfare state effect can therefore be considered to be significantly influenced by levels of subjective general health.
Previous studies highlight the need for promoting health in order to improve life satisfaction, especially in economically disadvantaged population groups [43]. There is a positive correlation between subjective health and the emotional dimension of wellbeing. Declining health status in old age does not always produce a similar decline in the emotional dimension of mental wellbeing [21]. Despite the majority of people reaching oldest old age with multiple health difficulties, they still tend to report high levels of self-reported health, which may potentially be a reflection of psychological adaptation [44].
It remains unclear whether the association between this positive subjective assessment and the strength of the welfare state is due to stronger psychological adaptation, fewer health-related concerns due to better access to public services or some other contextual factor linked to the welfare state. Although the exact mechanisms are objects for further studies, the end result remains the same, which is higher levels of mental wellbeing among the oldest old in stronger welfare states.
Similarly, the considerable influence of subjective health in relation to the psychological functioning dimension of mental wellbeing was demonstrated by the welfare state effect diminishing significantly (achieving full mediation) when subjective health was added to the mediation model. Positive subjective health appears to therefore support the psychological functioning dimension of mental wellbeing in oldest old age. Items making up the psychological functioning dimension have links to aspects of autonomy. Previous research also emphasised the importance of autonomy in older age, although highlighting that support may be necessary in accordance with age-related needs [45].
Therefore, welfare states that support health in oldest old age may simultaneously support a sense of autonomy. The flow dimension of mental wellbeing was also mediated by the subjective health variable and was higher in countries with stronger welfare state support. Supporting subjective health appears to therefore strengthen a feeling of meaning and engagement in oldest old age. Importantly, previous research underlined the ability derive enjoyment from daily activities despite health difficulties [46].
Subjective general health almost fully mediated the welfare state effect on the relational dimension. A good level of health was also found to be a protective factor against loneliness for people aged 60 years and over in a previous study [47]. Relationships are particularly important in oldest old age considering the higher risk of isolation and loneliness, for example, via the age-related losses of partners, siblings or friends [48].

4.2. Coping with Income and Mental Wellbeing

The basic purpose of the welfare state is to provide economic security and social equality, for example via social transfers, such as pensions. The current study found the welfare state effect on all dimensions of mental wellbeing to be mediated by the coping with income variable. Interestingly, the coping with income variable increased most with each incremental increase in WSI. Although research suggests that older adults report comparatively less financial difficulty than younger age groups, welfare state support appears to be especially important for individuals with higher support needs [49]. Income redistribution is one important aspect of welfare states; however, an equally important aspect of welfare states is access to non-income benefits and services.
The coping with income variable had the strongest impact on the evaluative and emotional dimension of mental wellbeing. Although using different measures of income and a younger age group, previous research indicated that income tends to influence the evaluative and the emotional dimensions differently [50]. Although specific reasons behind these differences lie outside of the scope of this article, it does appear that the welfare state effect is mediated by income for both dimensions also in the oldest old age group. Coping with one’s current income mediates the welfare state effect on the psychological functioning dimension, implying that that having a subjective sense of financial security impacts autonomy and a sense of accomplishment in oldest old age. A welfare state can support autonomy by ensuing that older adults have sufficient income to access what they need for a good life.
A preference for ‘ageing in place’—that is, remaining living in the home environment as long as possible [51], has been found to foster independence and allow for higher levels of control and a higher quality of life [52]. A welfare state can facilitate ageing in place by ensuring adequate income, suitable housing options, access to services and social support within and outside of the home [53]. It is equally important to ensure autonomy within the context of residential care in terms of people’s decisions about their daily activities, maintaining dignity and upholding human rights [54,55]. Welfare state support is especially important in relation to high support needs. Financial deterrents are more likely in countries where individuals are expected to make their own contributions to institutional care, which tend to only take place only when it is absolutely necessary [56].
The mediation effect on the flow dimension illustrates how a subjective sense of financial security influences feelings of being interested, absorbed and enthusiastic. Longitudinal benefits of economic wellbeing in younger age groups indicates that higher income and/or increases in income are associated with higher levels of certain aspects of psychological wellbeing, such as purpose in life, self-acceptance, personal grown and environmental mastery, paralleling findings from the current study [57]. Ensuring access to leisure activities in oldest old age may be done by ensuring adequate transport and mobility [58] and access to day centres [59] or leisure activities [60].
The coping with income variable almost fully mediated the welfare state effect on the relational dimension suggesting that a subjective sense of financial security promotes a sense of being treated with respect, of being appreciated as well as providing and receiving help and support. The relational dimension was least affected by differing levels of welfare state support (see Figure 3). The link between having an adequate income and the relational dimension seems logical in the oldest old age considering that oldest old age is a time when support needs are likely to increase [9]. With less support from the welfare state, family relations and family financial support become critical, especially in the context of inadequate income levels. Intergenerational family support is more likely to be unbalanced and non-reciprocal in low-income contexts. Circumstances in which the older adult is predominantly the recipient of support from family members has been linked to lower levels of life satisfaction [61].

4.3. Place in Society and Mental Wellbeing

The positive association between status and subjective well-being in the general population is well-documented in previous research [62]. Subjective social status—that is, how people perceive their place in society—has been reported to have links to health and mental health outcomes [63] also in old age [64]. These associations have been found to persist even when statistically accounting for socioeconomic indicators, such as income, education and occupational prestige confirming the usefulness of the subjective measure [65]. Furthermore, cross country comparisons suggest that the health benefits related to high levels of subjective social status appears to be slightly more prominent in more affluent countries [65].
Welfare generosity (represented here by higher WSI values) aims to decrease poverty and increase equity by redistributing resources through social welfare and social protection. The current study showed a strong welfare state effect for the place in society variable, indicating that participants from countries with a higher WSI also reported a higher level of perceived placement in society. The place in society variable had a significant indirect effect on all mental wellbeing dimensions, implying a broad influence on the welfare state effect in this regard. A positive correlation between status and subjective wellbeing was previously reported in an age group under the age of 80 [66]. High levels of subjective social-economic status have also been reported in relation to mental wellbeing in older age groups, although less attention has been given to individual dimensions [67,68].
The current study found the place in society variable to explain part of the welfare state in terms of the evaluative dimension. Similarly, actions aiming to improve social status (for example by reducing social inequality) have been found to improve life satisfaction in the general population [69]. The current study also found place in society to influence the emotional dimension. Our findings parallel previous studies reporting the subjective position in society to be an important predictor of happiness in the general age group [24]. This was also found to be moderated by the welfare state, although to a lesser extent in the context of the Nordic countries compared to Eastern European countries [24].
The place in society variable also mediated the welfare state effect on the psychological functioning dimension in the current study. More equitable welfare states with higher levels of resources can be assumed to support the psychological functioning dimension of mental wellbeing. This is of particular relevance in older age as this age group tends to be ascribed low social status, and ageist attitudes are pervasive in many countries [70]. Ageism and associated negative health outcomes have been found to be more prevalent in countries with lower level of resources [71] and a high percentage of older adults [72].
The place in society item also mediated the welfare state effect on the flow dimension, which could potentially be stimulated by a variety of community activities, including voluntary or charity work, educational or training courses, sport, social or other kind of clubs and political or community-related organisations. All of these have been shown to follow a social gradient, with a higher social position increasing the likelihood of engagement in community activities among older people [73]. Studies also indicate that facilitating these actions impacts how engaged older adults are in these activities [74]. Furthermore, the place of society variable fully mediated the welfare state effect the relational dimension, highlighting an association between subjective social status and a sense of being treated with respect, feeling appreciated and being able to provide help and support.
The current study implies that life conditions in the daily environment has a mediating effect on many dimensions of mental wellbeing in oldest old age. Although three main variables were picked out in more detail, other variables also paint a similar picture. The variable ‘learn new things’ mediated the welfare state effect on all dimensions of mental wellbeing. Life-long learning is an area of limited attention in the oldest old age group. The results from this study could potentially provide a springboard for further study.

4.4. Protective Factors for Mental Wellbeing in Oldest Old Age; Some Policy Considerations

Building age-friendly environments may require increased attention to the smaller details in life. The World Health Organisation (WHO) highlights the need for facilitating access to activities, such as outdoor environments, social participation including community exchange and lifelong learning, social and neighbourhood cohesion and civic engagement also in old age [75]. By pinpointing the importance of different life conditions for mental wellbeing, the current study gains insight and support for policy actions, which develop mentally healthy environments for the oldest old. The results also highlight the need for a Mental Health in all Policies (MHiAP) [76] approach and the importance of involving multiple sectors, such as health, long-term care, transport, housing, labour and social services and actions on different levels, such as governmental and statutory services, civil society as well as individuals, communities and families [77,78].
The results from the current study also emphasise the multidimensional nature of mental wellbeing and emphasize the welfare state influence on different dimensions. Facilitating the opportunity to engage in actions that support mental wellbeing in all ages and circumstances needs to be driven by supportive policies. Interestingly, all three of the intervening variables discussed here centre around the notion of autonomy. Autonomy is an area highlighted in the MWIA, including enhancing control, facilitating participation and promoting inclusion (see Table A2). Support autonomy in oldest old age is an important endeavour for welfare states, considering that this age group is at increased risk of being excluded from both services and social contacts [79].

4.5. Strengths and Limitations

A strength of the study is its focus on an often neglected age group, which is an especially important area of attention considering the ongoing demographic transition. The current study accounts for the complex nature of both welfare state and mental wellbeing by a multidimensional approach using a composite measure of welfare state (WSI) and including five dimensions of mental wellbeing.
There are however some limitations to be taken into consideration. First, the cross-sectional nature of the ESS data increases the risk of cohort effects. ESS-data were collected from the community (as opposed to supported living environments) meaning there is a bias towards data from people with higher levels of mental wellbeing. Depending on the welfare state structure, individuals with higher health needs are generally more likely to live in institutional care, and it is important to recognise that these individuals are not included in the current data set. Subsequent analyses using longitudinal data could provide further information on cohort and age effects in relation to mental wellbeing and welfare state effects.
Furthermore, the data used in this study are relatively old, stemming from 2012, as this is the last point when the ESS module relating to Subjective and Social Wellbeing was included. It is worth acknowledging that both WS measures and their association to mental wellbeing at different ages may have changed since then. The stability of our results should be tested in later research also looking at longitudinal effects. It would also be useful to repeat these analyses should the ESS repeat the rotating modules used in this study.
It is important to recognise that various perspectives exist in terms of mediation analysis and that statistical models used to test mediation are simply predictive or descriptive and cannot be considered to be inherently causal [80]. In the case of the current study, we can only conclude that the intervening variables may have an influence in terms of the welfare state effect, but we cannot imply causality. Multiple mediators can be assumed to lie behind the welfare state effect.

5. Conclusions

The current study develops our understanding of how life conditions in the daily environment mediate the effect of the welfare state on mental wellbeing among the oldest old. The welfare state impacts on health are broad and multifaceted, and our study confirms that the higher the level of welfare state, the higher the levels of all mental wellbeing dimensions also among the oldest old. Although many of the intervening variables had a mediating effect on the different dimensions of mental wellbeing, subjective health, coping with income and place in society showed the highest mediating effects for the welfare state variable for all dimensions of mental wellbeing. For the psychological functioning and relational dimensions, this mediation effect was close to complete or was complete. All three variables centre around supporting aspects of autonomy.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.C.-S., J.K. and K.W.; methodology, software and formal analysis, J.K.; writing—original draft preparation, J.C.-S.; writing—review and editing, J.C.-S., J.K. and K.W.; visualization, J.K. and J.C.-S.; supervision, K.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The support of the Joint Programming Initiative More Years Better Lives is gratefully acknowledged. Johanna Cresswell-Smith would like to thank Finska Läkaresällskapet for funding the opportunity to complete her PhD.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge contributions from the entire EMMY project. Open access funding provided by University of Helsinki.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Summary of mental wellbeing dimensions.
Table A1. Summary of mental wellbeing dimensions.
EvaluativeEmotionalFunctionalFlowRelational
How satisfied with lifeX
How happy are youX
Enjoyed life X
Were happy X
Felt calm and peaceful X
Had lot of energy X
Free to decide how to live my life X
Feel accomplishment X
What I do is valuable and worthwhile X
There are lots of things I am good at X
Feel very positive about myself X
Always optimistic about my future X
Interested X
Absorbed X
Enthusiastic X
Provide help and support X
Feel people treat you with respect X
Feel appreciated X
Receive help and support X
Table A2. Intervening variable allocation into MWIA protective factors.
Table A2. Intervening variable allocation into MWIA protective factors.
MWIA Protective FactorIntervening Variable
Enhancing controlSubjective general health
Coping with income
Hampered in daily life
Increasing resilience and community assetsAppreciate surroundings
Trust in people
Facilitating participationVoted
Voluntary work
Social activities
Promoting inclusionPlace in society
Learn new things
Chance to show capabilities
Table A3. Variables used in factor analyses of welfare state.
Table A3. Variables used in factor analyses of welfare state.
VariableDescription
GDP per capitaGross Domestic Product per capita in PPS, 2012, Index (EU28 = 100), Eurostat
Health/GDP + Social/GDPHealth expenditure as % of GDP, the World Bank DataBank, plus Social protection expenditure as % of GDP, 2012, Eurostat
Gender empowerment measureHuman Development Report 2009, United Nations (compiled by the ESS Multilevel Data Repository)
Life expectancy at 65Life expectancy in absolute value at 65 (average for male and female), 2012, Eurostat
Duration working lifeDuration of working life, 2012, Eurostat
GINIGini coefficient of equivalised disposable income (scale from 0 to 100), EU-SILC survey, 2012, Eurostat
IndividualismIndividualism versus Collectivism Index, Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. London: Sage Publications, https://www.hofstede-insights.com/product/compare-countries/. Description: The high side of this dimension, called Individualism, can be defined as a preference for a loosely-knit social framework in which individuals are expected to take care of only themselves and their immediate families. Its opposite, Collectivism, represents a preference for a tightly-knit framework in society in which individuals can expect their relatives or members of a particular in-group to look after them in exchange for unquestioning loyalty. A society’s position on this dimension is reflected in whether people’s self-image is defined in terms of “I” or “we.”
TrustAverage rating of trust by domain (Trust police, Trust legal system, Trust political system, Trust others), both sexes, all educational attainment levels, 16 years or over- (rating 0–10), EU-SILC survey, 2013, Eurostat, http://appsso.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/nui/show.do?dataset=ilc_pw03

Appendix B. Appendix Figures: Mediation Diagrams for Intervening Variables

Figure A1. Subjective health intervening variable. (a) Evaluative dimension. (b) Emotional dimension. (c) Functional dimension. (d) Flow dimension. (e) Relational dimension. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001.
Figure A1. Subjective health intervening variable. (a) Evaluative dimension. (b) Emotional dimension. (c) Functional dimension. (d) Flow dimension. (e) Relational dimension. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 19 04363 g0a1aIjerph 19 04363 g0a1b
Figure A2. Coping with income intervening variable: (a) Evaluative dimension. (b) Emotional dimension. (c) Functional dimension. (d) Flow dimension. (e) Relational dimension. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001.
Figure A2. Coping with income intervening variable: (a) Evaluative dimension. (b) Emotional dimension. (c) Functional dimension. (d) Flow dimension. (e) Relational dimension. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 19 04363 g0a2
Figure A3. Place in society intervening variable: (a) Evaluative dimension. (b) Emotional dimension. (c) Functional dimension. (d) Flow dimension. (e) Relational dimension. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001.
Figure A3. Place in society intervening variable: (a) Evaluative dimension. (b) Emotional dimension. (c) Functional dimension. (d) Flow dimension. (e) Relational dimension. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 19 04363 g0a3

References

  1. Andersen-Ranberg, K.; Petersen, I.; Robine, J.-M.; Christensen, K. Who Are the Oldest-Old? In Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe: First Results from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement i Europe; Börsch-Supan, A., Brugiavini, A., Jürges, H., Mackenbach, J., Siegrist, J., Weber, G., Eds.; Mannheim Research Institute for the Economics of Aging: Mannheim, Germany, 2005; pp. 35–40. [Google Scholar]
  2. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. World Population Prospects 2019: Data Booket; ST/ESA/SER.A/424. Available online: https://population.un.org/wpp/publications/ (accessed on 31 March 2022).
  3. Beard, J.R.; Officer, A.; de Carvalho, I.A.; Sadana, R.; Pot, A.M.; Michel, J.-P.; Lloyd-Sherlock, P.; Epping-Jordan, J.E.; (Geeske) Peeters, G.M.E.E.; Mahanani, W.R.; et al. The World Report on Ageing and Health: A Policy Framework for Healthy Ageing. Lancet 2016, 387, 2145–2154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Huppert, F.A. Psychological Well-being: Evidence Regarding Its Causes and Consequences. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2009, 1, 137–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Chida, Y.; Steptoe, A. Positive Psychological Well-Being and Mortality: A Quantitative Review of Prospective Observational Studies. Psychosom. Med. 2008, 70, 741–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Fried, L.P. Investing in Health to Create a Third Demographic Dividend. Gerontologist 2016, 56 (Suppl. S2), S167–S177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Knapp, M.; McDaid, D.; Parsonage, M. Mental Health Promotion and Mental Illness Prevention: The Economic Case. Available online: http://www.pssru.ac.uk/ (accessed on 21 June 2021).
  8. Schneider, S.M. Income Inequality and Subjective Wellbeing: Trends, Challenges, and Research Directions. J. Happiness Stud. Interdiscip. Forum Subj. Well-Being 2016, 17, 1719–1739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kalseth, J.; Cresswell-Smith, J.; Forsman, A.K.; Miret, M. Exploring the Association between Welfare State and Mental Wellbeing in Europe: Is There an Age Gradient? 2022, unpublished.
  10. Linton, M.-J.; Dieppe, P.; Medina-Lara, A. Review of 99 Self-Report Measures for Assessing Well-Being in Adults: Exploring Dimensions of Well-Being and Developments over Time. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e010641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Diener, E. Subjective Well-Being. Psychol. Bull. 1984, 95, 542–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Diener, E. Guidelines for National Indicators of Subjective Well-Being and Ill-Being. J. Happiness Stud. 2006, 7, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Dolan, P.; Metcalfe, R. Measuring Subjective Wellbeing: Recommendations on Measures for Use by National Governments. J. Soc. Policy 2012, 41, 409–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Kahneman, D.; Diener, E.; Schwarz, N. (Eds.) Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  15. Ryff, C.D.; Singer, B.H. Know Thyself and Become What You Are: A Eudaimonic Approach to Psychological Well-Being. J. Happiness Stud. 2008, 9, 13–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Keyes, C.L.M. The Mental Health Continuum: From Languishing to Flourishing in Life. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2002, 43, 207–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Keyes, C.L.M. Promoting and Protecting Mental Health as Flourishing: A Complementary Strategy for Improving National Mental Health. Am. Psychol. 2007, 62, 95–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Seligman, M.E.P. Flourish: A Visionary New Understanding of Happiness and Well-Being; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  19. Cresswell-Smith, J.; Amaddeo, F.; Donisi, V.; Forsman, A.K.; Kalseth, J.; Martin-Maria, N.; Miret, M.; Walhbeck, K. Determinants of Multidimensional Mental Wellbeing in the Oldest Old: A Rapid Review. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2019, 54, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Lara, E.; Martín-María, N.; Forsman, A.K.; Cresswell-Smith, J.; Donisi, V.; Ådnanes, M.; Kaasbøll, J.; Melby, L.; Nordmyr, J.; Nyholm, L.; et al. Understanding the Multi-Dimensional Mental Well-Being in Late Life: Evidence from the Perspective of the Oldest Old Population. J. Happiness Stud. 2020, 21, 465–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Steptoe, A.; Deaton, A.; Stone, A.A. Psychological Wellbeing, Health and Ageing. Lancet 2015, 385, 640–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Kieny, C.; Flores, G.; Ingenhaag, M.; Maurer, J. Healthy, Wealthy, Wise, and Happy? Assessing Age Differences in Evaluative and Emotional Well-Being Among Mature Adults from Five Low- and Middle-Income Countries. Soc. Indic. Res. 2022, 1019–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Diener, E.; Diener, M.; Diener, C. Factors Predicting the Subjective Well-Being of Nations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 69, 851–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ejrnæs, A.; Greve, B. Your Position in Society Matters for How Happy You Are. Int. J. Soc. Welf. 2017, 26, 206–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Eikemo, T.A.; Bambra, C.; Joyce, K.; Dahl, E. Welfare State Regimes and Income-Related Health Inequalities: A Comparison of 23 European Countries. Eur. J. Public Health 2008, 18, 593–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Huppert, F.A.; So, T.T.C. Flourishing Across Europe: Application of a New Conceptual Framework for Defining Well-Being. Soc. Indic. Res. 2013, 110, 837–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Allen, J.; Balfour, R.; Bell, R.; Marmot, M. Social Determinants of Mental Health. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2014, 26, 392–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Cooke, A.; Friedli, L.; Coggins, T.; Edmonds, N.; Michaelson, J.; O’Hara, K.; Snowden, L.; Stansfield, J.; Steuer, N.; Scott-Samuel, A. Mental Well-being Impact Assessment, 3rd ed.; National MWIA Collaborative: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  29. Miret, M.; Cabello, M.; Marchena, C.; Mellor-Marsá, B.; Caballero, F.F.; Obradors-Tarragó, C.; Haro, J.M.; Ayuso-Mateos, J.L. The State of the Art on European Well-Being Research within the Area of Mental Health. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2015, 15, 171–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Forsman, A.K.; Stengård, E.; Wahlbeck, K. Enhancing Mental Health and Mental Wellbeing in Older People. In nterventions and Policies to Enhance Well-being; Huppert, F.A., Cooper, C.L., Eds.; WileyBlackwell: Oxford, UK, 2014; pp. 331–354. [Google Scholar]
  31. European Social Survey Round 6 Data. Data File Edition 2.4. NSD—Norwegian Centre for Research Data, Norway—Data Archive and Distributor of ESS Data for ESS ERIC. 2012. Available online: https://www.europeansocialsurvey.org/data/conditions_of_use.html (accessed on 4 April 2022).
  32. Brown, T.A. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  33. Donisi, V.; Tedeschi, F.; Gonzalez-Caballero, J.L.; Cresswell-Smith, J.; Lara, E.; Miret, M.; Forsman, A.K.; Wahlbeck, K.; Amaddeo, F.; Kalseth, J. Is Mental Well-Being in the Oldest Old Different from That in Younger Age Groups? Exploring the Mental Well-Being of the Oldest-Old Population in Europe. J. Happiness Stud. 2021, 22, 1693–1717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Esping-Andersen, G. 4 The Three Political Economies of the Welfare State. Int. J. Sociol. 1990, 20, 92–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Barnish, M.; Tørnes, M.; Nelson-Horne, B. How Much Evidence Is There That Political Factors Are Related to Population Health Outcomes? An Internationally Comparative Systematic Review. BMJ Open 2018, 8, e020886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Deeming, C.; Hayes, D. Worlds of Welfare Capitalism and Wellbeing: A Multilevel Analysis. J. Soc. Policy 2012, 41, 811–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ebbinghaus, B.; Whiteside, N. Shifting Responsibilities in Western European Pension Systems: What Future for Social Models? Glob. Soc. Policy 2012, 12, 266–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Jorm, A.F.; Ryan, S.M. Cross-National and Historical Differences in Subjective Well-Being. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2014, 43, 330–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Shalev, M. Limits and Alternatives to Multiple Regression in Comparative Research. In Capitalisms Compared; Mjøset, L., Clausen, T.H., Eds.; Comparative Social Research; Emerald Group Publishing Limited: Bentley, UK, 2007; Volume 24, pp. 261–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Preacher, K.J.; Zyphur, M.J.; Zhang, Z. A General Multilevel SEM Framework for Assessing Multilevel Mediation. Psychol. Methods 2010, 15, 209–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Jaul, E.; Barron, J. Age-Related Diseases and Clinical and Public Health Implications for the 85 Years Old and Over Population. Front. Public Health 2017, 5, 335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Högberg, B.; Strandh, M.; Baranowska-Rataj, A.; Sevä, I.J. Ageing, Health Inequalities and the Welfare State: A Multilevel Analysis. J. Eur. Soc. Policy 2018, 28, 311–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Moreno-Agostino, D.; Wu, Y.-T.; Daskalopoulou, C.; Hasan, M.T.; Huisman, M.; Prina, M. Global Trends in the Prevalence and Incidence of Depression:A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 281, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Araujo, L.; Ribeiro, O.; Paúl, C. Hedonic and Eudaimonic Well-Being in Old Age through Positive Psychology Studies: A Scoping Review. An. Psicol. 2017, 33, 568–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Nordmyr, J.; Creswell-Smith, J.; Donisi, V.; Lara, E.; Martín-María, N.; Nyholm, L.; Forsman, A.K. Mental Well-Being among the Oldest Old: Revisiting the Model of Healthy Ageing in a Finnish Context. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Health Well-Being 2020, 15, 1734276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Jarosz, E. What Makes Life Enjoyable at an Older Age? Experiential Wellbeing, Daily Activities, and Satisfaction with Life in General. Aging Ment. Health 2021, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Nyqvist, F.; Victor, C.R.; Forsman, A.K.; Cattan, M. The Association between Social Capital and Loneliness in Different Age Groups: A Population-Based Study in Western Finland. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Dykstra, P.A. Older Adult Loneliness: Myths and Realities. Eur. J. Ageing 2009, 6, 91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Litwin, H. Social networks and well being in socio-geographical context: A comparison of older people in mediterranean and non-mediterranean european countries. Gerontologist 2009, 49, 488. [Google Scholar]
  50. Kahneman, D.; Deaton, A. High Income Improves Evaluation of Life but Not Emotional Well-Being. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 16489–16493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Pani-Harreman, K.E.; Bours, G.J.J.W.; Zander, I.; Kempen, G.I.J.M.; van Duren, J.M.A. Definitions, Key Themes and Aspects of ‘Ageing in Place’: A Scoping Review. Ageing Soc. 2021, 41, 2026–2059. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Khalaila, R.; Vitman-Schorr, A. Internet Use, Social Networks, Loneliness, and Quality of Life among Adults Aged 50 and Older: Mediating and Moderating Effects. Qual. Life Res. Int. J. Qual. Life Asp. Treat. Care Rehabil. 2018, 27, 479–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Iecovich, E. The Impact of Organizational Qualities of Old Age Homes in Israel on the Subjective Well-Being of Female Residents. J. Soc. Work Long-Term Care 2002, 1, 37–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Harrefors, C.; Sävenstedt, S.; Axelsson, K. Elderly People’s Perceptions of How They Want to Be Cared for: An Interview Study with Healthy Elderly Couples in Northern Sweden. Scand. J. Caring Sci. 2009, 23, 353–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Moilanen, T.; Kangasniemi, M.; Papinaho, O.; Mynttinen, M.; Siipi, H.; Suominen, S.; Suhonen, R. Older People’s Perceived Autonomy in Residential Care: An Integrative Review. Nurs. Ethics 2021, 28, 414–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Costa-Font, J. Housing-Related Well-Being in Older People: The Impact of Environmental and Financial Influences. Urban Stud. 2013, 50, 657–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kaplan, G.A.; Shema, S.J.; Leite, C.M.A. Socioeconomic Determinants of Psychological Well-Being: The Role of Income, Income Change, and Income Sources during the Course of 29 Years. Ann. Epidemiol. 2008, 18, 531–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Böcker, L.; van Amen, P.; Helbich, M. Elderly Travel Frequencies and Transport Mode Choices in Greater Rotterdam, the Netherlands. Transportation 2017, 44, 831–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Orellana, K.; Manthorpe, J.; Tinker, A. Day Centres for Older People—Attender Characteristics, Access Routes and Outcomes of Regular Attendance: Findings of Exploratory Mixed Methods Case Study Research. BMC Geriatr. 2020, 20, 158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Rivera-Torres, S.; Mpofu, E.; Jean Keller, M.; Ingman, S. Older Adults’ Mental Health Through Leisure Activities During COVID-19: A Scoping Review. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 2021, 7, 23337214211036776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Lowenstein, A.; Katz, R.; Gur-Yaish, N. Reciprocity in Parent-Child Exchange and Life Satisfaction among the Elderly: A Cross-National Perspective. J. Soc. Issues 2007, 63, 865–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Schneider, S.M. Why Income Inequality Is Dissatisfying—Perceptions of Social Status and the Inequality-Satisfaction Link in Europe. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2019, 35, 409–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hoebel, J.; Lampert, T. Subjective Social Status and Health: Multidisciplinary Explanations and Methodological Challenges. J. Health Psychol. 2020, 25, 173–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Präg, P.; Mills, M.C.; Wittek, R. Subjective Socioeconomic Status and Health in Cross-National Comparison. Soc. Sci. Med. 2016, 149, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Demakakos, P.; Nazroo, J.; Breeze, E.; Marmot, M. Socioeconomic Status and Health: The Role of Subjective Social Status. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008, 67, 330–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Hadjar, A.; Samuel, R. Does Upward Social Mobility Increase Life Satisfaction? A Longitudinal Analysis Using British and Swiss Panel Data. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 2015, 39, 48–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Navarro-Carrillo, G.; Alonso-Ferres, M.; Moya, M.; Valor-Segura, I. Socioeconomic Status and Psychological Well-Being: Revisiting the Role of Subjective Socioeconomic Status. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Read, S.; Grundy, E.; Foverskov, E. Socio-Economic Position and Subjective Health and Well-Being among Older People in Europe: A Systematic Narrative Review. Aging Ment. Health 2016, 20, 529–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Clench-Aas, J.; Holte, A. The Financial Crisis in Europe: Impact on Satisfaction with Life. Scand. J. Public Health 2017, 45 (Suppl. S18), 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Vauclair, C.-M.; Marques, S.; Lima, M.L.; Bratt, C.; Swift, H.J.; Abrams, D. Subjective Social Status of Older People across Countries: The Role of Modernization and Employment. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2015, 70, 650–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Chang, E.-S.; Kannoth, S.; Levy, S.; Wang, S.-Y.; Lee, J.E.; Levy, B.R. Global Reach of Ageism on Older Persons’ Health: A Systematic Review. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0220857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Marques, S.; Mariano, J.; Mendonça, J.; De Tavernier, W.; Hess, M.; Naegele, L.; Peixeiro, F.; Martins, D. Determinants of Ageism against Older Adults: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2020, 17, 2560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Niedzwiedz, C.L.; Richardson, E.A.; Tunstall, H.; Shortt, N.K.; Mitchell, R.J.; Pearce, J.R. The Relationship between Wealth and Loneliness among Older People across Europe: Is Social Participation Protective? Prev. Med. 2016, 91, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Hank, K.; Erlinghagen, M. Dynamics of Volunteering in Older Europeans. Gerontologist 2010, 50, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. World Health Organisation. Age-Friendly Environments in Europe. A Handbook of Domains for Policy Action; Online, 2017; p. 160. Available online: https://www.euro.who.int/en/publications/abstracts/age-friendly-environments-in-europe.-a-handbook-of-domains-for-policy-action-2017 (accessed on 31 March 2022).
  76. Botezat, I.; Campion, J.; Garcia-Cubillana, P.; Guðmundsdóttir, D.G.; Halliday, W.; Henderson, N.; Holte, A.; Santos, M.J.H.; Japing, K.; Katschnig, H.; et al. Joint Action on Mental Health and Well-Being, Mental Health in All Policies. Situation Analysis and Recommendations for Action. 2017. Available online: https://arneholte.no/psykisk-helse/syv-prinsipper-og-tjueni-grep-for-et-psykisk-sunnere-norge/ (accessed on 15 October 2019).
  77. Hartfree, Y.F.; Hirsch, D.; Sutton, L. Minimum Income Standards and Older Pensioners’ Needs; Joseph Rowntree Foundation: York, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  78. Marmot, M.; Allen, J.J. Social Determinants of Health Equity. Am. J. Public Health 2014, 104 (Suppl. S4), S517–S519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Key, W.; Culliney, M. The Oldest Old and the Risk of Social Exclusion. Soc. Policy Soc. 2018, 17, 47–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Agler, R.; De Boeck, P. On the Interpretation and Use of Mediation: Multiple Perspectives on Mediation Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Mediation diagram denoting WSI effect on mental wellbeing with and without mediating aspects of WSI.
Figure 1. Mediation diagram denoting WSI effect on mental wellbeing with and without mediating aspects of WSI.
Ijerph 19 04363 g001
Figure 2. Country WS Index by WS Regime type for 24 European countries.
Figure 2. Country WS Index by WS Regime type for 24 European countries.
Ijerph 19 04363 g002
Figure 3. The total effect of WSI on mental wellbeing (coefficient and 95-percent confidence intervals) (left panel) and mental wellbeing-predictions for different values of WSI (right panel).
Figure 3. The total effect of WSI on mental wellbeing (coefficient and 95-percent confidence intervals) (left panel) and mental wellbeing-predictions for different values of WSI (right panel).
Ijerph 19 04363 g003
Table 1. Mediator variables.
Table 1. Mediator variables.
Intervening Variable aMeanSDCV bWSI Effect c N d
Subjective general health (1–5)3.10.930.310.411 ***2053
Feeling about household’s income currently (1–4)2.90.900.310.574 ***2034
Hampered in daily activities by illness/disability/infirmity/mental problems (1–3)2.20.750.340.114 *2055
Take notice of and appreciate your surroundings (0–10)7.22.220.310.155 **1985
Most people can be trusted or you cannot be too careful (0–10)5.02.640.530.336 ***2044
Voted last national election (0–1)0.80.420.550.183 ***2048
Involved in work for voluntary or charitable organisations, how often past 12 months (1–6)1.71.420.860.310 ***2048
Take part in social activities compared to others of same age (1–5)2.51.100.450.241 ***1968
Place in society (0–10)5.11.930.380.364 ***1935
Learn new things in life (0–6)2.81.880.660.266 ***1996
Little chance to show how capable I am (1–5)2.91.030.350.0381951
a Intervening variable from ESS, numbers in parentheses show the range of the variable with low values corresponding to low ratings. b CV = coefficient of variation= standard deviation/mean. c WSI effect in this table is for standardised values (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) to be comparable between variables. d Missing values for items used to calculate the mental wellbeing measures implies a lower N than in the initial sample. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01 and * p < 0.05. Means, standard deviations (SD), coefficient of variation (CV), coefficient for WSI effect on intervening variable (path a in Figure 1), and number of observations.
Table 2. Mental wellbeing dimensions.
Table 2. Mental wellbeing dimensions.
EvaluativeEmotionalFunctionalFlowRelational
mean3.803.163.763.754.27
SD0.860.920.630.890.63
CV *0.230.290.170.240.15
N **20231969188619281922
* CV = coefficient of variation = standard deviation/mean. ** Missing values for items used to calculate the mental wellbeing measures implies a lower N than in the initial sample. Means, standard deviations (SD), coefficient of variation (CV), and number of observations (N).
Table 3. Coefficient for WSI without (top row) and with control for intervening variable *.
Table 3. Coefficient for WSI without (top row) and with control for intervening variable *.
EvaluativeEmotionalFunctionalFlowRelational
Total effect of WSI (c)0.402 ***0.252 ***0.147 ***0.218 ***0.107 ***
Effect of WSI (c’) after control for:
Subjective general health0.273 *** (0.000)0.092 *** (0.000)0.056 (0.000)0.095 * (0.000)0.043 (0.000)
Coping with income0.232 ***(0.000)0.122 *** (0.000)0.069 ** (0.000)0.146 *** (0.000)0.037 (0.000)
Hampered in daily life0.380 *** (0.013)0.219 *** (0.012)0.128 *** (0.013)0.193 *** (0.014)0.095 ** (0.017)
Appreciate surroundings0.350 *** (0.006)0.203 *** (0.006)0.110 *** (0.006)0.143 ** (0.005)0.060 *(0.006)
Trust in people0.353 *** (0.000)0.213 *** (0.000)0.133 *** (0.010)0.198 *** (0.015)0.079 *** (0.000)
Voted0.383 *** (0.001)0.231 *** (0.001)0.133 *** (0.001)0.198 *** (0.001)0.092 ** (0.002)
Voluntary work0.368 *** (0.000)0.211 *** (0.000)0.116 *** (0.000)0.181 *** (0.000)0.089 ** (0.001)
Social activities0.360 *** (0.000)0.186 *** (0.000)0.100 ** (0.000)0.161 *** (0.000)0.078 * (0.000)
Place in society0.291 *** (0.000)0.129 *** (0.000)0.063 * (0.000)0.100 ** (0.000)0.032 (0.000)
Learn new things0.348 *** (0.000)0.181 *** (0.000)0.095 ** (0.000)0.132 * (0.000)0.067 * (0.000)
Chance to show capabilities0.402 *** (0.435)0.236 *** (0.433)0.138 *** (0.435)0.199 *** (0. 434)0.103 ** (0.435)
* Control for age and gender in all regressions. Significance level denoted by stars as follows: *** p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01 and * p < 0.05. Number in parentheses show the p-value for the mediator test (indirect effect).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cresswell-Smith, J.; Wahlbeck, K.; Kalseth, J. Life Conditions as Mediators of Welfare State Effect on Mental Wellbeing among Oldest Old in Europe. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 4363. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074363

AMA Style

Cresswell-Smith J, Wahlbeck K, Kalseth J. Life Conditions as Mediators of Welfare State Effect on Mental Wellbeing among Oldest Old in Europe. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(7):4363. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074363

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cresswell-Smith, Johanna, Kristian Wahlbeck, and Jorid Kalseth. 2022. "Life Conditions as Mediators of Welfare State Effect on Mental Wellbeing among Oldest Old in Europe" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 7: 4363. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19074363

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop